| ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Can Factors Related to Self-Regulated Learning |
Variable | M | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 |
Final Grade | 86.36 | 13.31 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
GPA | 3.01 | 0.63 | .40*** |
|
|
|
|
|
Computer Self-Efficacy | 6.38 | 1.18 | -.09 | -.25*** | (70) |
|
|
|
Intrinsic Goal Orientation | 13.11 | 3.11 | .10 | .03 | .07 | (62)[74] |
|
|
Eff/Resource Management | 30.83 | 6.02 | .32*** | .25*** | .09 | .28*** | (80)[73] |
|
Expectancy | 23.50 | 3.88 | .39*** | .30*** | .10 | .23** | .50*** | (85)[81] |
GPAXExpectancy | 71.08 | 20.56 | .52*** | .84*** | -.13 | .15* | .45*** | .66*** |
Note. N = 201. Reliability estimates appear on the diagonal. Estimates in parentheses are for the current sample after factor analysis, those in brackets are from original researchers’ instruments.
*p< .05, **p< .01 , ***p< .001
Survey participants fell into three categories according to their reason for taking an online course during the spring 2005 semester. Of the respondents, 47.8 percent (n = 96) stated that learning online was more convenient for them than taking a traditional face-to-face course, while 33.8 percent (n = 68) reported that they had no option. “No option” meant that at the time the student registered, either the course was only offered online or there were no face-to-face course sections available. A smaller number of students, 18.4 percent (n = 37) gave a reason related to their curiosity or interest in learning via the electronic medium. See Table 2.
Reason | n | M | SD | Low | High |
Convenience | 96 | 84.83 | 15.97 | 0 | 100 |
No Other Option | 68 | 87.75 | 8.46 | 63 | 100 |
Interest in Online Learning | 37 | 87.78 | 12.86 | 31 | 99 |
Student responses to two survey questions about self-efficacy for the use of computer technology were added together and the sum represented the student’s overall self-report score for computer self-efficacy (M = 6.38, SD = 1.18). Prior academic college achievement was measured using the current semester GPA. The mean GPA for the sample was 3.01 and the SD 0.63. See Table 1.
Interaction term. In the current study, GPA and expectancy for learning were found to be moderately correlated (r = .3) (see Table 1). This r value as well as literature-based evidence for their positive correlation5,7,14-17 drove the decision to create an interaction term, consisting of the cross product of the variable that measured prior college academic achievement (GPA) and the variable that measured individual expectancy for learning (ExpSE_sum). This new variable was labeled GPA_Exp and was included in the predictive model.18
Mean standard deviations, Pearson correlations, and coefficient alpha reliability estimates for the study’s independent variables appear in Table 1. Coefficients ≥.1 were considered indicative of a correlation between a particular predictor variable and the dependent variable. Therefore, based on this criterion, the following bivariate correlations revealed five predictor variables significantly related to learning achievement: (a) interaction of GPA and expectancy (r = .52), (b) prior college achievement as measured by GPA (r = .40), (c) expectancy (r = .39), and (d) effort regulation (r = .32. All of these correlations were significant at least at p < .05, and all were in the predicted directions.
Using multiple regression, final course grades were regressed on the linear combination of all the variables in the model. These seven variables included (a) prior academic achievement (GPA), (b) computer self-efficacy (Comp_SE), (c) intrinsic goal orientation (IGO_sum), (d) resource management (TPEffreg), (e) expectancy (ExpSE_sum), (f) reason for taking an online course (reason_ol), and (g) the interaction between GPA and expectancy (GPA_Exp). Table 3 depicts the prediction of final grade based on the full model.
| b | SE | b | t | p |
Intercept | -74.91 | 26.64 |
| -2.81 | .005 |
Grade Point Average | 43.80 | 8.92 | 2.09 | 4.91 | .0001 |
Computer Self-efficacy | -0.21 | 0.67 | -.02 | -0.32 | .75 |
Intrinsic Goal Orientation | -0.07 | 0.25 | -.01 | -0.26 | .80 |
Resource Management | 0.10 | 0.15 | .05 | 0.69 | .49 |
Expectancy | 5.66 | 1.06 | 1.65 | 5.33 | .0001 |
Reason_ol | 2.29 | 1.51 | .09 | 1.51 | .13 |
Grade point average X Expectancy | -1.52 | 0.36 | -2.35 | -4.22 | .0001 |
Full Model: F 7, 193 = 17.12, p = .0001. adj R2 = .361.
The linear combination of the independent variables significantly predicted final course grade in asynchronous undergraduate online courses (adj. R2 = .36, p<.001). See Table 3. Three of the seven independent variables were significant (P<.0001) predictors of undergraduate learning achievement in asynchronous online courses; these predictors were prior college learning achievement (GPA), expectancy for learning (ExpSE_sum), and the interaction of prior college learning achievement with expectancy for learning (GPA_Exp).
The magnitude of contribution for each significant predictor was determined by its associated standardized regression coefficient (Table 3); they were GPA (2.1, p<.0001), expectancy (1.7, p<.0001), and GPA_Exp (-2.3, p<.0001). The differences between the absolute values for these three coefficients did not appear to be widely divergent. Thus, it appeared that none of these three independent variables had a greater effect than the others in predicting the dependent variable.
Interaction term. Figure 1 compares the relationship between the independent variable (Exp) and the dependent variable (Fin Gr) where GPA is low (below the median) and where GPA is high (above the median). This figure demonstrates that when GPA is below the median, the slope of expectancy for learning on FinGr is steeper than when GPA is equal to or greater than the median. This suggests that expectancy for learning exerts a greater effect at lower values of GPA than it does at higher values of GPA. Furthermore, this figure also suggests that GPA’s impact on final course grade is mitigated at higher levels of expectancy for learning.
In this study, the best predictors of learning achievement in undergraduate asynchronous online courses were prior college academic achievement (GPA), expectancy for learning (ExpSE_sum), and the interaction term based on the cross product of prior academic achievement and expectancy (GPA_Exp). In addition to being the most important independent variables in the model, these three variables also correlated most strongly with the dependent variable compared to other independent variables in the model.
The study’s results yielded a parsimonious solution to the original study research question and indicated that although there were several SRL sub factors that were bivariately correlated with learning, only one turned out to be a predictor of learning achievement in asynchronous online undergraduate courses. For example, although effort regulation was fairly correlated with FinGr (r = .32), it was more highly correlated with the other self-regulated learning subfactor, expectancy (r = .50). Therefore, it appears that effort regulation probably shared variance in common with expectancy and, as a result, was a weaker predictor of final grade than was the expectancy for control of learning subfactor. As a result, expectancy acted as an “umbrella” term that represented the other SRL correlates of the dependent variable (FinGR) in the predictive model of learning achievement in asynchronous online undergraduate courses.
This study’s findings suggest that expectancy, or an expectation that one will experience positive outcomes in one’s learning, is a central driving force for self-regulation.16, 19 Moreover, Bandura and others have underscored the role played by individual self-efficacy in facilitating expectancy for learning.14, 20 Therefore, an individual with strong expectancy for learning possesses the “can do” attitude required to succeed in learning. Such an attitude is the product of positive reinforcement and explains the mutually positive or synergistic relationship not only between prior academic achievement and expectancy, but also between expectancy and other self-regulated learning subfactors.
For example, it would appear that because expectancy was the only subfactor to make it into the predictive model, it acted as a global factor or “proxy” that represented the other SRL sub-factor (effort regulation) in the predictive model. This observation is reasonable because strong expectancy for learning depends on having other positive attitudes and behaviors consistent with success in learning. It is as though once an individual expects positive outcomes for his learning and takes responsibility for his learning, he will do what it takes (such as regulate his effort accordingly and apply appropriate time and study management strategies) in order to be a successful learner.
Thus, it is unlikely that a multiple regression equation that already contained expectancy would need other variables like effort regulation in order to improve the accuracy of its predictive power; any variance in final grade due to effort regulation had probably already been accounted for by expectancy. As a result, the effort regulation variable was redundant and consequently displayed a non-significant beta weight.
College has traditionally been a stage of education where individuals must assume greater responsibility for their learning compared to the primary and secondary schooling experiences.21, 22 Moreover, today’s college student is faced with an even greater need to be able to assume responsibility for his learning because more undergraduate courses and programs are being delivered via asynchronous online environments. This study’s findings suggest that individuals with the greatest expectancy for learning, regardless of their prior academic achievement, were the most successful asynchronous online learners. Nevertheless, expectancy for learning appears to be a learner characteristic that is molded and shaped by previous academic learning experiences.14, 19, 23 Therefore, in order to ensure academic learning success, it behooves responsible educators to ensure that students who enter college are armed with strong expectancy for controlling their learning.
1. Employ other research methodologies in order to study the role that SRL plays in explaining learning achievement in online courses. Researchers are calling for mixed methods research because the SRL construct, is recognized as a complex entity and, as such, may require a range of methodologies in order to better understand its influence on learning.21, 24 For example, it is known that triangulation of qualitative and quantitative methods can help confirm a theory to a greater degree than can either method used in isolation.25 Thus, findings from both quantitative research methodologies based on survey data collection and qualitative research strategies based on interviewing and observation can help to either confirm or contradict hypotheses regarding the connection between self-regulated learning and learning achievement. Therefore, a future replication of this study should include qualitative as well as quantitative techniques of data collection and analysis.
2. Prior learning experiences influence individual expectancy for control of learning. Therefore it is important to investigate how factors external to the learner may facilitate expectancy and self regulation of learning. For example, future studies of learner achievement in asynchronous online courses can be designed to investigate how instructor and/or instructional design factors may impact self regulation of learning and learner achievement.
3. Finally, repeat this study with a larger sample of undergraduate students. As more undergraduate programs move toward online course delivery, it is useful to understand which learner-associated factors influence learning achievement in asynchronous Web-based courses.
Turnbull, E.D. “Investigating the Potential for Improving Experiential Undergraduate Curriculum through the Concept of Personality.” Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Western Sydney, Sydney, 2003.
Hartley, K. and Bendixen, L. “Educational Research in the Internet Age: Examining the Role of Individual Characteristics.” Educational Researcher 30(9), (2001, December): 22-26.
Kember, D. “Beliefs about Knowledge and the Process of Teaching and Learning as a Factor in Adjusting to Study in Higher Education.” Studies in Higher Education 26, no. 2 (2001): 205-221.
Zimmerman, B. J. “Attaining Self-regulation: A Social Cognitive Perspective.” In M. Boekarts, P. R. Pintrich, M. Zeidner (Editors). Handbook of Self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 2000: 13-35.
Bandura, A. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 1986.
Ormrod, J. Human Learning. Third edition. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall, 1999.
Zimmerman, B. J. “Developing Self-fulfilling Cycles of Academic Regulation: An Analysis of Exemplary Instructional Models.” In Schunk, D. H. and B. J. Zimmerman, (Editors). Self-regulated Learning: From Teaching to Reflective Practice. Guilford Press: New York, 1998: 1-19.
Garavilia, L. S. and M. E Gredler. “Prior Achievement, Aptitude and Use of Learning Strategies as Predictors of College Student Achievement.” College Student Journal 36, no. 4 (2002): 616-625.
Pintrich, P. R. and E. V. De Groot. “Motivational and Self-regulated Learning Components of Classroom Academic Performance.” Journal of Educational Psychology 82, no. 3 (1990): 3-40.
Pressley, M. and E. S. Ghatala. “Self-regulated Learning: Monitoring Learning from Text.” Educational Psychologist 25, no. 1 (1990): 19-33.
Schunk, D. H. “Self-regulation of Self-efficacy and Attributions in Academic Settings.” In Schunk, D. H. and B. J. Zimmerman, (Editors). Self-regulation of Learning and Performance: Issues and Educational Implications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1994: 75-99.
Williams, J. E. and C. M. Hellman. “Investigating Self-regulated Learning among First Generation Community College Students.” Journal of Applied Research in the Community College 5, no. 2 (1998): 83-87.
Pintrich, P. R. et al. A Manual for the Use of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan, 1991: 3.
Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: W. H. Freeman & Company, 1997.
Pajares, F. “Gender and Perceived Self-efficacy in Self-regulated Learning.” Theory into Practice 41, no. 2 (2002): 116-125.
VanZile-Tamsen, C. “The Predictive Power of Expectancy of Success and Task Value for College Students’ Self-regulated Strategy Use.” Journal of College Student Development 42, (2001): 233-241.
Zimmerman, B. J. “Dimensions of Academic Self-regulation: A Conceptual Framework for Education.” In Schunk, D. H. and B. J. Zimmerman (Editors), Self-regulation of Learning and Performance: Issues and Educational Applications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1994: 3-21.
Jaccard, J and R. Turrisi. “Interaction Effects in Multiple Regression,” Second edition. Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences (Series). Thousand Oaks CA: Sage publications, 2003.
Boekarts, M., P. R. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner. Handbook of Self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 2000.
Williams, P. E. and C. M. Hellman. “Differences in Self-regulation for On-line Learning between First and Second Generation College Students.” Research in Higher Education 45, no. 1 (2004): 71-82.
Perry, R. P. “Academic Control and Action Control in the Achievement of College Students: A Longitudinal Field Study.” Journal of Educational Psychology 93, no. 4 (2001): 776-789.
Thompson, B. R. and P. R. Geren. “Classroom Strategies for Identifying and Helping College Students at Risk for Academic Failure.” College Student Journal 36, no. 3 (2002): 398-402.
Hofer, B., S. Yu, and P. Pintrich. “Teaching College Students to Be Self-regulated Learners.” In Schunk, D. H. and B. J. Zimmerman (Editors), Self-regulated Learning from Teaching to Self-reflective Practice. New York: The Guilford Press, 1998: 57-85.
Patrick, H. and M. J. Middleton. “Turning the Kaleidoscope: What We See When Self Regulated Learning Is Viewed with a Qualitative Lens.” Educational Psychologist 3 (2002): 27-39.
Risjord, M.W., S. B. Dunbar, and M. F. Maloney. “A New Foundation for Methodological Triangulation.” Journal of Nursing Scholarship 34, no. 3 (2002): 269-276.
Demographic items
1. What is your e-mail ID?
2. Gender: Male, Female
3. Age_____
4. Ethnic background (Non-Hispanic Black, Hispanic, Non-Hispanic White. Asian/Pacific Islander, Native American, Mixed Race)
5. Class level (freshman, sophomore, junior, senior)
6. What is the name and section number of the Web-based course that you are taking this semester? (if taking more than one course, complete this for the course that is required for either a major or minor course of study).
7. Is this course taught 100 percent online with no scheduled on-campus sessions?
8. Why are you taking this course online as opposed to in a campus-based classroom?
9. The item that best describes how I feel about my ability to overcome computer- and technology-related problems:
1. Not at all confident
2. Somewhat confident
3. Confident
4. Very confident
10. The item that best describes how I feel about my ability to use computer technology such as the Internet, e-mail, and chat:
1. Not at all confident
2. Somewhat confident
3. Confident
4. Very confident
Self-regulation of Learning Items
Please indicate how true each of the following statements is of you. There are no right or wrong answers, just answer as accurately as possible. Use the scale below to answer the questions. If you think a statement is very true of you, select “7”; if
a statement is not at all true of you, then select 1. If the statement is more or less true of you then find the number between 1 and 7 that best describes you.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Not at all true of me Very true of me
11. In a class like this, I prefer course material that really challenges me so I can learn new things.
12. If I study in appropriate ways, then I will be able to learn the material in this course.
13. I believe I will receive an excellent grade in this class.
14. During the times that I am logged onto the course site, I often miss important points because I am thinking of other things.
15. I usually study in a place where I can concentrate on my course work.
16. I often feel so lazy or bored when I study that I quit before I finish what I planned to do.
17. I prefer course material that arouses my curiosity, even if it is difficult to learn.
18. It is my own fault if I don’t learn the material in this course.
19. I’m confident I can do an excellent job on the assignments and tests in this course.
20. When I become confused about something I’m reading for class, I go back and try to
figure it out.
21. I make good use of my study time for this course.
22. I work hard to do well in class even if I don’t like what we are doing.
23. The most satisfying thing for me is trying to understand the content as thoroughly as possible.
24. If I try hard enough, then I will understand the course material.
25. I expect to do well in this class.
26. I often find that I have been reading for class but don’t know what it was all about.
27. I find it hard to stick to a study schedule.
28. When course work is difficult, I give up or only study the easy parts.
29. When I have the opportunity, I choose course assignments that I can learn from even if they don’t guarantee a good grade.
30. If I don’t understand the course material, it is because I didn’t try hard enough.
31. Considering the difficulty of this course, the online format, and my skills, I think that I will do well in this class.
32. I try to think through a topic and decide what I am supposed to learn from it rather than just reading it over when studying.
33. I rarely find time to review my notes or readings before an exam.
34. Even when course materials are dull and uninteresting, I manage to keep working until I finish.
Paul D. Bell, PhD, RHIA, CTR, is Associate Professor of Health Services and Information Management in the School of Allied Health Sciences at East Carolina University in Greenville, NC. Email: bellp@ecu.edu
Duane Akroyd, PhD, RT(R), is Associate Professor of Higher Education and Director of Graduate Programs in the Department of Adult and Community College Education at North Carolina State University. Email: duane_akroyd@ncsu.edu